The Hechinger Report is a national nonprofit newsroom that reports on one topic: education. Sign up for our weekly newsletters to get stories like this delivered directly to your inbox. Consider supporting our stories and becoming a member today.

Research has shown both short and long-term benefits of high-quality pre-K. So it was surprising when a recent study found that children who attended Tennessee’s state-run voluntary pre-K program actually performed slightly worse on sixth grade tests and behavioral measures than children who were wait-listed for the program.

This single study has renewed the debate about the value of universal pre-K and the need for continued investments in early learning.

Yet, as noted by researchers and advocates alike, there are many reasons why the study’s findings aren’t really so bleak — and they all highlight the need to collect better data so we can understand what really works.

First, the state-run program was studied over a decade ago, and it was not particularly strong — it would not meet basic indicators of quality if evaluated today.

Second, we lack systematic data on the wait-listedchildren — we do not know whether these children went on to attend a different pre-K program and, if they did, how high-quality those experiences were.

Third, researchers have yet to examine how the children’s subsequent school experiences may have affected the results, a significant limitation given earlier work finding that the program’s short-term impacts varied considerably depending on the quality of the children’s elementary school teachers.

And fourth, we still don’t know how children who participated in the program are faring in later adolescence or will fare in adulthood: Other studies of pre-K attendees have detected long-term effects on outcomes like educational attainment, employment and earnings, even after observing some short-term skill fade-out.

Related: Behind the findings of the Tennessee pre-K study that found negative effects for graduates

Only with more and better data will we be able to know where this pre-K program and others go right and where they go wrong.  Better data on pre-K enrollment will help us make better comparisons.

We already know that the positive impacts of pre-K are largest when children in the comparison group have stayed at home.  Many of the children who were wait-listed for Tennessee’s program likely attended other high-quality pre-K programs, such as Head Start.  

Overall, we also need better data on quality. Since 2010, there has been tremendous growth in our understanding and measurement of high-quality pre-K, and Tennessee has strengthened its program through marked investments in curricula and teacher professional development.

Only with more and better data will we be able to know where this pre-K program and others go right and where they go wrong.

Over the last decade, Tennessee has also encouraged the collection of classroom quality data via the Classroom Assessment Scoring System. The state is trying to determine where further improvement is needed. Other programs, such as Head Start and initiatives in Louisiana and New York City, have made similar investments in measurement tools to strengthen data on quality. Yet many pre-K systems are still relying on quality indicators that do not factor the influence of  interactions with teachers or the use of evidence- and play-based curricula and rich instructional content.

With better data on classrooms, collected consistently across the full range of pre-K programs, policymakers will better understand the quality of existing programs and how to strengthen the components that matter most.

The measures of academic outcomes used in the Tennessee study represented a narrow band of children’s skills. They did not capture competencies like problem-solving and executive functioning — admittedly more difficult to teach and assess — that high-quality pre-K programs have been shown to enhance. There is growing evidence that such skills are more likely to be sustained as children move through elementary school.

Related: OPINION: What might the future of universal pre-K look like? As researchers, we have some concerns

Most data systems for early learning are not easily linked to children’s elementary school experiences and longer-term outcomes. Even in the latest Tennessee results, we don’t know how children’s experiences in elementary school and beyond relate to their pre-K experiences.

For example, the research team found that children assigned to the Tennessee pre-K program were substantially more likely to be referred to special education services starting in kindergarten. But given challenges in linking pre-K to later special education data, it is difficult to know whether this outcome was good or bad for the children. Did it help get them the services they needed, or was the referral to these services actually harmfulfor their learning and development?

By investing in data systems that connect early learning to K-12, policymakers will be able to better interpret study results.

Working families want high-quality care for their young children. And many states are interested in expanding their programs. Rather than curbing investments in pre-K, the Tennessee results raise important considerations about how these programs should be designed and demonstrate the importance of high-quality data for making smart policy decisions and investments that pay off for kids and families.

Meghan McCormick is a senior research associate and JoAnn Hsueh is the director of the Families & Children’s Development policy area at MDRC.

This story about education data was produced by The Hechinger Report, a nonprofit, independent news organization focused on inequality and innovation in education. Sign up for Hechinger’s newsletter.

The Hechinger Report provides in-depth, fact-based, unbiased reporting on education that is free to all readers. But that doesn't mean it's free to produce. Our work keeps educators and the public informed about pressing issues at schools and on campuses throughout the country. We tell the whole story, even when the details are inconvenient. Help us keep doing that.

Join us today.

Letters to the Editor

1 Letter

At The Hechinger Report, we publish thoughtful letters from readers that contribute to the ongoing discussion about the education topics we cover. Please read our guidelines for more information. We will not consider letters that do not contain a full name and valid email address. You may submit news tips or ideas here without a full name, but not letters.

By submitting your name, you grant us permission to publish it with your letter. We will never publish your email address. You must fill out all fields to submit a letter.

  1. To the editor,
    My colleague, Mark Lipsey, and I have grave concerns about the opinion piece published by McCormick and Hsueh providing their analysis of our study, the only randomized control trial of a statewide pre-k program with long term follow up. In January we published the results through 6th grade. We believe that our results warrant attention and discussion. However the characterizations of our findings by McCormick and Hsueh are at best misleading and often quite wrong. We have published an extensive set of our concerns and corrections and invite Hechinger readers to examine them: https://my.vanderbilt.edu/tnprekevaluation/files/2022/06/Response-to-The-Hechinger-Report-McCormick-Hsueh_6-8-22.pdf
    Thank you,
    Dale Farran

Submit a letter

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *